19 research outputs found

    A method to predict overall food preferences

    Get PDF
    Most natural ecosystems contain animals feeding on many different types of food, but it is difficult to predict what will be eaten when food availabilities change. We present a method that estimates food preference over many study sites, even when number of food types vary widely from site to site. Sampling variation is estimated using bootstrapping. We test the precision and accuracy of this method using computer simulations that show the effects of overall number of food types, number of sites, and proportion of missing prey items per site. Accuracy is greater with fewer missing prey types, more prey types and more sites, and is affected by the number of sites more than the number of prey types. We present a case study using lion (Panthera leo) feeding data and show that preference vs prey size follows a bell-curve. Using just two estimated parameters, this curve can be used as a general way to describe predator feeding patterns. Our method can be used to: test hypotheses about what factors affect prey selection, predict preferences in new sites, and estimate overall prey consumed in new sites.The Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and a Hugh Kelly Fellowship from Rhodes University, Grahamstown, SA.http://www.plosone.orgdm2022Mammal Research Institut

    Changes of movement patterns from early dispersal to settlement

    Get PDF
    Moving and spatial learning are two intertwined processes: (a) changes in movement behavior determine the learning of the spatial environment, and (b) information plays a crucial role in several animal decision-making processes like movement decisions. A useful way to explore the interactions between movement decisions and learning of the spatial environment is by comparing individual behaviors during the different phases of natal dispersal (when individuals move across more or less unknown habitats) with movements and choices of breeders (who repeatedly move within fixed home ranges), that is, by comparing behaviors between individuals who are still acquiring information vs. individuals with a more complete knowledge of their surroundings. When analyzing movement patterns of eagle owls, Bubo bubo, belonging to three status classes (floaters wandering across unknown environments, floaters already settled in temporary settlement areas, and territory owners with a well-established home range), we found that: (1) wandering individuals move faster than when established in a more stable or fixed settlement area, traveling larger and straighter paths with longer move steps; and (2) when floaters settle in a permanent area, then they show movement behavior similar to territory owners. Thus, movement patterns show a transition from exploratory strategies, when animals have incomplete environmental information, to a more familiar way to exploit their activity areas as they get to know the environment better. © Springer-Verlag 2009.Peer Reviewe

    Density of animal locations vs. distance from edge of patch, for three different types of behaviors to the edge, for straight edges.

    No full text
    <p>Note the log scale for density. Densities are in proportion to the mean density in the whole patch - thus, 1 means no effect of the edge. Attraction and avoidance give proportionally similar effects, with most effects within 1 unit from the edge.</p

    Example of densities of animals at different types of edge shapes, for width of shapes = 10.

    No full text
    <p>Example of densities of animals at different types of edge shapes, for width of shapes = 10.</p

    Directionality towards the inside, where 1 means pointed towards the inside of the patch, and -1 means pointed towards the outside.

    No full text
    <p><b>A</b>: Overall effects for different edge behaviors and edge shapes. Bars are grouped by behavior to edge, and within each group, bars show animals' behaviors to the edge: straight, in and out. The main differences are in the type of avoidance behavior, not in edge shapes. <b>B</b>: For the Out shape only, To-inside vs. width of edge shape. Each color represents different edge behaviors. With all behaviors, directionality changes very close to the tips of the edge shapes.</p

    The simulated habitat patch that modeled animals traveled in.

    No full text
    <p>The grey areas show the sampling sites for the three different kinds of treatment edges. Size and shape of the edge types was varied.</p

    Density of animal locations vs. widths of edge shapes. Densities are in proportion to the mean density in the whole patch - thus, 1 means no effect of the edge.

    No full text
    <p>The three plots show different types of behaviors to the edge: <b>A</b> - attraction, <b>B</b> - neutral, <b>C</b> - avoidance. Within each graph, the different colors lines represent the different types of edge shapes. The different lines of each color represent different narrownesses. The tight clustering of the colored lines as compared to the change along the x-axis shows that most of the effects on density are due to point width, not point narrowness.</p

    Three types of modeled behaviors to edges.

    No full text
    <p>The top row of figures represents how animals reflected from the edge and the bottom row represents how animals turned while within detecting distance of the edge.</p

    Changes of movement patterns from early dispersal to settlement

    Get PDF
    Moving and spatial learning are two intertwined processes: (a) changes in movement behavior determine the learning of the spatial environment, and (b) information plays a crucial role in several animal decision-making processes like movement decisions. A useful way to explore the interactions between movement decisions and learning of the spatial environment is by comparing individual behaviors during the different phases of natal dispersal (when individuals move across more or less unknown habitats) with movements and choices of breeders (who repeatedly move within fixed home ranges), that is, by comparing behaviors between individuals who are still acquiring information vs. individuals with a more complete knowledge of their surroundings. When analyzing movement patterns of eagle owls, Bubo bubo, belonging to three status classes (floaters wandering across unknown environments, floaters already settled in temporary settlement areas, and territory owners with a well-established home range), we found that: (1) wandering individuals move faster than when established in a more stable or fixed settlement area, traveling larger and straighter paths with longer move steps; and (2) when floaters settle in a permanent area, then they show movement behavior similar to territory owners. Thus, movement patterns show a transition from exploratory strategies, when animals have incomplete environmental information, to a more familiar way to exploit their activity areas as they get to know the environment better. © Springer-Verlag 2009.Peer Reviewe

    How fledglings explore surroundings from fledging to dispersal. A case study with Eagle Owls Bubo bubo

    No full text
    Movement strategies are one of the primary mechanisms underlying animal survival. Despite the impressive amount of studies on animal movement during successive stages in the life history in birds, little is known about the characteristics of movements during the post-fledging dependence period. Such a period represents a crucial phase of the life history during which individuals can show important displacements around the natal nest. Here we present path search strategies employed by radio-tagged fledgling Eagle Owls Bubo bubo during the post-fledging dependence period (1962 locations from 41 tagged owlets). During the post-fledging dependence period, individuals show dynamic movement behaviours that might be related to the development of individuals' mobility and cognitive abilities. Immediately after leaving the nest, individuals focused their activities close to the nest, but after a few weeks individuals travelled further and faster, up to the moment that they started to disperse. At the time that the mobility of owlets was highest, their movements described straighter paths. Movement patterns were not affected by morphologicalor physiological parameters of body condition. Between-sibling distances showed a significant interaction between sex and time: until 20 days after owlets left the nest, siblings were closer together, independently of the sex. Between-sibling distances increased with time as individuals became increasingly mobile; just before the start of dispersal, family units seemed to dissolve.Peer Reviewe
    corecore